Most Recent Links
Follow us on Facebook or subscribe to our mailing list, to receive news updates. Learn more.
By Christopher Shea ATTENTION, NEW PARENTS. Jerome Kagan, a professor emeritus of psychology at Harvard, has devised a fun little test for you. At 4 months old, plop your baby into a bouncy seat and present him with a series of colorful new toys - ones he's never seen - one after the other, for 20 seconds at a time. Does he cry madly and shake his arms and legs? If yes, be forewarned: Your baby may be at higher risk for "developing serious anxiety over social interactions" a decade down the road. If he screams at 4 months, he'll be more likely to stay home from junior-high dances. If he screams, he'll be more likely to answer "no" when a psychologist asks, at age 11, "Are you happy most of the time?" It won't really matter if you cuddled your child as an infant or showered him with play dates as a toddler. He'll probably never be a brash CEO or politician, although he might become a brilliant solitary researcher or a melancholy poet. On the other hand, if your baby just stares calmly at the toys, he will be calm on dates but also slightly more likely to become a delinquent, because parental threats won't faze him. © Copyright 2004 Globe Newspaper Company.
Keyword: Development of the Brain
Link ID: 6089 - Posted: 06.24.2010
by Arline Kaplan, Psychiatric Times August 2004 Vol. XXI Issue 9 Although eating disorders have been considered to be largely sociocultural in origin, findings from family, twin and molecular genetic studies conducted during the last decade are refuting that perspective, an expert in genetic epidemiology told attendees at the recent 2nd World Congress on Women's Mental Health in Washington, D.C. (Bulik et al., 2004). "Twenty years ago when I started in this field, and gave my favorite lecture on eating disorders, it was all about the role of the family and social factors in the etiology of eating disorders," said Cynthia M. Bulik, Ph.D., William R. and Jeanne H. Jordan Distinguished Professor of Eating Disorders in the department of psychiatry and director of the eating disorders program at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. "Both anorexia and bulimia were very much viewed as disorders of choice. These young girls were viewed as trying to emulate some cultural ideal and diet themselves down to a certain weight. Now, any patient would have told you had you listened that wasn't what they were doing. They went far beyond any societal ideal in Cosmopolitan or any other magazine." Bulik explained that when she and colleagues started talking about genes as being involved in these disorders, "people pretty much thought we were out of our minds." However, the investigators are discovering a complex interplay between genes and the environment leading to the development of anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN). © 2004 Psychiatric Times
Keyword: Anorexia & Bulimia
Link ID: 6088 - Posted: 06.24.2010
by Amir Raz, Ph.D., Psychiatric Times August 2004 Vol. XXI Issue 9 In the United States, approximately 2% to 6% of school-age children are diagnosed with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Data show that stimulant medication is the most consistently successful form of treatment in ADHD, with psychostimulants such as dextroamphetamine (Adderall, Dexedrine) and methylphenidate (Ritalin) exerting therapeutic effects via modulation of the noradrenergic and dopaminergic systems. In the United States, methylphenidate is used to treat over 2 million children with ADHD annually. Methylphenidate acts primarily by blocking the dopamine transporter and increasing extracellular dopamine in the striatum. Despite more than 50 years of clinical and neuroscientific research, appropriate diagnostic and therapeutic interventions for ADHD are still an issue for many people, and although reports summarize the current knowledge (American Academy of Pediatrics Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Committee on Quality Improvement, 2001), they use parameters that are still based on the same descriptive determinations that have plagued the field for years. Stimulants are thus widely prescribed for the treatment of ADHD even though the mechanisms subserving their calming effects are not easily understood. The past two decades have ushered in a new era of methodological advances in tools for noninvasive imaging of the living brain. Brain imaging has forged an impressive link between psychology, psychiatry and neuroscience. The information gleaned from such advances has been used to study both the anatomical and functional aspects of neural processing. © 2004 Psychiatric Times.
Keyword: ADHD
Link ID: 6087 - Posted: 06.24.2010
by Antonio Mantovani, M.D., Ph.D., and Sarah H. Lisanby, M.D. Psychiatric Times August 2004 Vol. XXI Issue 9 Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a non-invasive means of stimulating focal regions of the brain using magnetic fields. Since its introduction in 1985, TMS has been used to study localization of brain functions, connectivity of brain regions and pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric disorders. The potential uses of TMS to treat psychiatric disorders are under active study. This article reviews the state of knowledge about the therapeutic potential of TMS in psychiatry. Transcranial magnetic stimulation is an investigational medical procedure performed by placing an electromagnetic coil on the scalp (Figure). High-intensity current is rapidly turned on and off in the coil through the discharge of a capacitor. This produces a time-varying magnetic field that lasts for about 100 to 200 microseconds. The magnetic field strength is about 1.5 to 2 tesla (about the same intensity as the static magnetic field used in clinical magnetic resonance imaging) at the surface of the coil, but the strength of the magnetic field drops off exponentially with distance from the coil. The proximity of the brain to the time-varying magnetic field results in current flow in neural tissue and in membrane depolarization. Transcranial magnetic stimulation is experimental; it is not approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration for the treatment of any disorder. A striking effect of TMS occurs when one places the coil on the scalp over the primary motor cortex. A single TMS pulse of sufficient intensity causes involuntary movement in the muscle represented by that region of cortex. © 2004 Psychiatric Times.
Keyword: Depression
Link ID: 6086 - Posted: 06.24.2010
Psychiatric Times August 2004 Vol. XXI Issue 9 It Is Reasonable To Try and Treat Depression in BD Primarily With Antidepressants by Lori Altshuler, M.D. Bipolar disorder (BD) affects approximately 1% of the population and is associated with a high morbidity and mortality (Goodwin and Jamison, 1990). Bipolar disorder is recurrent in almost all cases, and most patients will spend more time in the depressed than the manic phase of their illness over their lifetime. This is true for patients with bipolar I disorder (BD-I) as well as bipolar II disorder (BD-II) (Goodwin and Jamison, 1990; Judd et al., 2002). Suicide attempts and completed suicides are high in this population (Goodwin and Jamison, 1990). Most Patients With BD Do Not Need, or Would Not Benefit From, Antidepressants by S. Nassir Ghaemi, M.D. Voltaire is reputed to have held his contemporary medical colleagues in low regard, saying: "Doctors pour drugs of which they know little, to cure diseases of which they know less, into human beings of whom they know nothing." There is, no doubt, a herd mentality, codified in the "standard-of-care" legal criterion, that physicians share with all of mankind. Progress in medicine depends, however, on the ability to critically examine one's assumptions and a willingness to apply standards of evidence that share at least some aspects of scientific method. © 2004 Psychiatric Times
Keyword: Schizophrenia; Depression
Link ID: 6085 - Posted: 06.24.2010
BU neurobiologists find evidence hippocampus in rat brain triggers special form of memory (Boston) -- For millennia, the process of memory and remembering has intrigued scholars and scientists. In 350 B.C., Aristotle, in his seminal treatise on the subject, described it as having two forms: familiarity and recollection. Of these, he considered recollection to be a purely human condition. That tenet is now being challenged by researchers at Boston University. Neurobiologists at Boston University's Center for Memory and Brain have provided the first evidence that rats use recollection when recognizing items they have recently experienced. In addition, the researchers show that rodents' capacity for recollection-like memory retrieval depends on the brain structure known as the hippocampus, the same structure believed to be involved in recollection in humans. Their findings are published in the September 9 issue of the journal Nature. Although neuroimaging studies of hippocampal activity in normal individuals as well as studies of amnesia indicate the hippocampus could be crucial to recollection, definitive methods for assessing hippocampal activity in memory have largely remained out of reach.
Keyword: Learning & Memory
Link ID: 6084 - Posted: 09.09.2004
By Anna Salleh, ABC Science Online — Next time you start imitating chimpanzees at the zoo, be aware that they know what you're doing. Australian researcher Mark Nielsen and colleagues at the University of Queensland report they have the first evidence that animals other than humans can recognize when they're being imitated. The research is published online ahead of print publication in the journal Animal Cognition. "We know that human children are able to recognize they're being imitated by around 14 months," Nielsen told ABC Science Online. "So we were interested to see if chimpanzees showed a similar ability." The researchers set up a series of videotaped experiments where one researcher, Emma Collier-Baker, imitated a friendly 31-year old male chimp called Cassie. The researchers looked for behavior normally seen in human children when they know someone is imitating them. Copyright © 2004 Discovery Communications Inc.
Keyword: Emotions
Link ID: 6083 - Posted: 06.24.2010
Two proteins may halt Parkinson’s disease, experiments with rats have shown, and work towards a potential human therapy has already begun. Researchers claim to have slowed the progress of Parkinson’s disease in rats by injecting two proteins into their brains. The proteins prevented the brain cell loss associated with the disease. The debilitating illness is caused by progressive brain cell death in a central part of the brain called the substantia nigra. These cells are responsible for producing the chemical dopamine, which acts on the basal ganglia - the part of the brain that controls movement. As more of these cells die, less dopamine is produced, giving rise to the characteristic movement problems associated with Parkinson’s disease. Until now, most therapies have concentrated on replenishing dopamine, but that does not stop the brain cell loss and so has had limited success. Now, working in rats, a team from Cedars-Sinai Medical Center in California, US, have succeeded in halting brain cell death in the vital dopamine-producing region. © Copyright Reed Business Information Ltd.
Keyword: Parkinsons
Link ID: 6082 - Posted: 06.24.2010
By CHERYL WITTENAUER, Associated Press Writer ST. LOUIS -- Scientists say they've identified a gene that appears to be linked to both alcoholism and depression, a finding that may one day help identify those at higher risk for the diseases and guide new treatments. Previous studies of twins and adopted siblings have suggested there likely are genes in common underlying alcoholism and depression, and that the two disorders seem to run in families. But the lead researcher of the new study says this is the first report of a specific gene that seems to increase risk for both disorders. "Clinicians have observed a connection between these two disorders for years, so we are excited to have found what could be a molecular underpinning for that association," said Alison Goate, the Washington University School of Medicine researcher who led the study. Follow-up research might help reveal the underlying biology that makes some people susceptible to alcoholism, others to depression, some to both diseases, and others to neither. Goate says a variation or alteration of the CHRM2 gene influences those four separate conditions. The study is published in the September issue of the journal Human Molecular Genetics. Copyright © 2004, The Associated Press
Keyword: Drug Abuse; Depression
Link ID: 6081 - Posted: 06.24.2010
Anyone who has looked at the aerial world during an underwater dive will have marvelled at the distortions in apparent size, shape, and position of aerial objects. In a new study, researchers have shown that archer fish can learn to cope with these strongly viewpoint-dependent distortions, and this ability enables the fish to precisely judge from any underwater viewpoint the absolute size of their aerial prey. The work is reported by Stefan Schuster, now at the University of Erlangen-Nuremburg, and colleagues at the University of Freiburg. Many animals are able to judge the absolute size of visual objects, a skill that becomes especially useful for tasks such as selecting prey. The ability to judge absolute size, however, requires that the visual system be able to perform a critical correction to account for distance between the eye and the viewed object: dramatic differences in the size of the actual retinal image occur as the distance between viewer and object changes. A situation in which this corrective requirement is especially challenging arises for animals that need to judge the size of aerial prey from an underwater vantage point. Here, as the study of Schuster and colleagues shows, the complex optical situation arising from distance and refraction requires precise understanding of the relationship between an object's apparent size and the fish's relative position to the object. In the experiments reported by the researchers, archer fish viewed a set of eight disks at various heights above the water surface from various horizontal distances and selected one of the disks as a shooting target based only on its absolute size. Although naïve fish often selected disks that would be too large to be swallowed, all could eventually learn to judge absolute size with great precision; in doing so, they perfectly accounted for the complex optical situation posed by their underwater viewpoint.
Keyword: Vision
Link ID: 6080 - Posted: 09.08.2004
Pushing neurons' physiological limits provides researchers with new ways to repair nerve damage (Philadelphia, PA) – Sometimes it is the extremes that point the way forward. Researchers at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine have induced nerve fibers – or axons – to grow at rates and lengths far exceeding what has been previously observed. To mimic extreme examples in nature and learn more about neuronal physiology, they have mechanically stretched axons at rates of eight millimeters per day, reaching lengths of up to ten centimeters without breaking. This new work has implications for spinal cord and nerve-damage therapy, since longer implantable axons are necessary for this type of repair. In the present study, the team, led by Douglas H. Smith, MD, Professor of Neurosurgery and Director of the Center for Brain Injury and Repair, placed neurons from rat dorsal root ganglia (clusters of nerves just outside the spinal cord) on nutrient- filled plastic plates. Axons sprouted from the neurons on each plate and connected with neurons on the other plate. The plates were then slowly pulled apart over a series of days, aided by a precise computer-controlled motor system. "By rapid and continuous stretching, we end up with huge bundles of axons that are visible to the eye," says Smith. The axons started at an invisible 100 microns and have been stretched to 10 centimeters in less than two weeks. Smith and colleagues report their findings in the cover story of the September 8, 2004 issue of the Journal of Neuroscience.
Keyword: Development of the Brain
Link ID: 6079 - Posted: 06.24.2010
London, UK: International experts will (Wednesday 8 September) consider the evidence for a link between the rise in childhood leukaemia and increased light at night at an international scientific conference in London. The incidence of childhood leukaemia increased dramatically in the twentieth century. The increase has mainly affected the under five age group, in whom the risk increased by more than 50 per cent during the second half of the century alone. Although the causes of leukaemia in children are poorly understood, environmental factors are thought to play a major role in the rising incidence since changes in our genetic make up simply do not happen on this kind of timescale. If this is the case, then it may be possible to take preventative measures, but first we need to determine what these factors are. Whilst the link between leukaemia and light at night may, on the surface, seem surprising, it has a logical basis and there is considerable evidence pointing towards the association. Compared with 100 years ago we are exposed to considerable light at night (LAN) during the natural hours of darkness. LAN disrupts our natural circadian rhythm, suppressing the normal nocturnal production of the hormone melatonin.
Keyword: Biological Rhythms
Link ID: 6078 - Posted: 09.08.2004
Helen Pilcher Ever wondered what makes parrots so good at mimicking human speech? It turns out that the feathered impressionists use their tongues to create vowel-like sounds, just as we do. In human speech, noise is produced in the larynx and can then be modified by the movement of the tongue in the mouth. This helps us to make complex vowel and consonant sounds. Until now, many researchers thought that birds produced and modified their song in the avian equivalent of the larynx, the syrinx, and that the tongue played no role at all. But parrots are known to bob their fleshy tongues back and forth when they talk, so Gabriel Beckers from Leiden University in the Netherlands and colleagues decided to see whether these movements contribute to the birds' great talent for mimicry. Their results are published in Current Biology1. The team studied five feral monk parakeets (Myiopsitta monachus), which had been caught and killed as part of a government pest control program in Florida. In each bird, they replaced the syrinx with a tiny electronic speaker and then used a hook to move the tongue around as the amplifier played bursts of sound. ©2004 Nature Publishing Group
Keyword: Language
Link ID: 6077 - Posted: 06.24.2010
I am lying on a couch on my side when suddenly the white wall opposite me becomes a mosaic. Alarmed, I get up and walk around. All the walls are mosaics. My world has been transformed into a cubist painting. I call my aunt in Manchester. She is a glaucoma sufferer who is constantly urging me to get my eyes checked in case I have inherited the disease. She has always warned me to be vigilant about any change in my vision in case it signaled impending blindness. I tell her what's happening. "Go to the doctor NOW!" she pleads. That was five years ago, near midnight, in provincial France. A relief GP was summoned. This young man, straight out of medical school, took my blood pressure. It was normal. But he told me to get to the nearest hospital as a precaution. There, the medics were also baffled. They suggested an urgent trip to the eye specialist. The next morning his diagnosis was: "Ophthalmic migraine. Nothing you can do but wait." It wore off and, since this attack, I've never seen mosaics. It left me wondering why none of the medics recognised the symptoms. And so I wanted more people to know about ophthalmic migraines. Ophthalmic migraines are not that rare, though the more classic storm-in-the-head type has been part of my life since my mid-twenties. Once the migraine starts, I yearn for a dark room and utter quiet for 12-36 hours. It begins with the sensation of a white-hot needle passing through my left eye and across the left side of my face. I can't bend my head or turn it without intense pain. I shiver and then sweat. Sometimes there are sneezing bouts and dizziness. Once, I fainted. ©2004 Independent Digital (UK) Ltd.
Keyword: Pain & Touch
Link ID: 6076 - Posted: 06.24.2010
Nearly half of people with epilepsy have problems learning something new and feel they cannot think quickly enough, a survey has found. An even higher proportion of respondents said that they regularly feel sleepy and lethargic. The survey, by the International Bureau for Epilepsy, found 56% of respondents blamed their problems on their medication. Many said it blighted work, personal relationships and leisure activities. Problems such as the inability to recall names and faces, or access memories, were often reported. Some said slowness of thought prevented them from completing their education, or securing rewarding jobs, and from fulfilling basic tasks such as remembering a shopping list, or a friend's telephone number. The results, based on a poll of 425 people with epilepsy in five European countries, appear to confirm long-standing concerns that epilepsy drugs do have a negative impact on brainpower. Hilary Mounfield, chair of the European Committee of the International Bureau for Epilepsy, said: "We hear of far too many people whose lives are blighted not only by their epilepsy, but also by the drugs they are prescribed to treat it. (C)BBC
Keyword: Epilepsy
Link ID: 6075 - Posted: 09.07.2004
By DONALD G. McNEIL Jr. One night, asking his wife about their dinner plans, Stephen R. Anderson, a Yale professor of linguistics and psychology, got the reply: "I want to go out." The next night, he found the cat clawing the rug near the door. His wife calmed him down by translating: "She's saying, 'I want to go out.' " So do we conclude, Dr. Anderson asks, that the cat can talk? The answer, he argues, is no - although, as a human, he could say the same thing by shaking his head, rolling his eyes or even saying sarcastically, "Yeah, sure." Dr. Anderson is the author of "Doctor Dolittle's Delusion," to be published by Yale University Press in November. The book is a linguistics-based argument that if kindly John Dolittle of Puddleby-on-the-Marsh was hearing voices, they weren't coming from Jip the dog or Gub-Gub the pig. The idea that animals have all-but-human mental lives, emotions and powers of communication has become increasingly fashionable, after centuries in which such notions were considered absurd. Since the 1970's, as animal behaviorists have trained apes to make requests by stringing gestures or ideograms together and acousticians have detected that both whales and elephants make subsonic calls, suspicions have arisen that animals have more to say than humans realized. Copyright 2004 The New York Times Company
Keyword: Language
Link ID: 6074 - Posted: 09.07.2004
Mystery of consciousness PHILIP MARCHAND Scientists who have been trying to understand the brain have recently tried to measure neural activity of Republicans and Democrats to see if political affiliations had anything to do with brain chemistry. The results were inconclusive. (I think the Democrat brains were more active in the "I feel your pain" part of the limbic system.) What really caught my eye about a New York Times Magazine article on the topic was the following statement: "One of the most celebrated insights of the past 20 years of neuroscience is the discovery — largely associated with the work of Antonio Damasio — that the brain's emotional systems are critical to logical decision-making. People who suffer from damaged or impaired emotional systems can score well on logic tests but often display markedly irrational behaviour in everyday life." I'm sure Damasio has done good work, rooting around the neocortex. But what does it say for neuroscience that one of its "most celebrated insights" is something we've known for three or four millennia? "Imagination does not breed insanity," G.K. Chesterton wrote around the beginning of the 20th century. "Exactly what does breed insanity is reason. Poets do not go mad; but chess players do." Copyright Toronto Star Newspapers Limited.
Keyword: Miscellaneous
Link ID: 6073 - Posted: 06.24.2010
We know them as heroes, but many of the first-responders who survived 9-11 felt far from heroic. Instead they were shattered by post-traumatic stress. As this ScienCentral News video reports, some software engineers scrambled to help. Stephen King, a retired New York City fire fighter, got out of the World Trade Center alive on September 11th, 2001. But the trauma didn't end that day. "It overtook every aspect of my life," says King. "I couldn't enjoy anything, I couldn't get basic necessities like sleep…I was like a walking zombie, literally. All the things that always kept me occupied and busy just nothing seemed to matter anymore." King reached out for help and found out he was suffering from post-traumatic stress disorder or PTSD. Psychiatrist JoAnn Difede, director of the Program for Anxiety and Traumatic Stress Studies and a psychiatry professor at Weill Medical College of Cornell University, began working with King using the first-line, or first choice treatment for PTSD, called imaginal exposure therapy. "We ask the patient to imagine the event as if it were occurring again in their own imagination," she explains. "You want the person to relive the experience and process the memories. If they can't access their memories then they really can't process it and get better." That's what happened to King. He felt he made some initial progress, then "hit a plateau." © ScienCentral, 2000- 2004.
Keyword: Stress
Link ID: 6072 - Posted: 06.24.2010
Newborn babies prefer to look at attractive faces, says a UK researcher, suggesting that face recognition is hardwired at birth, rather than learned. Alan Slater and his colleagues at the University of Exeter showed paired images of faces to babies as young a one day old and found that they spent more time fixated on the more attractive face. “Attractiveness is not in the eye of the beholder, it’s innate to a newborn infant,” says Slater. Developmental psychologists have known for years that babies have preferences for certain objects, such as high contrast images, and curvy, biological shapes. But where these preferences come from remained unknown. Slater’s research, using extraordinarily young infants, supports the idea that babies are not mere blank slates, but instead come into the world with a fairly well developed perception system. © Copyright Reed Business Information Ltd.
Keyword: Vision; Sexual Behavior
Link ID: 6071 - Posted: 06.24.2010
BLOOMINGTON, Ind. -- When it comes to making noise, both parrots and humans rely on extremely specialized vibrating organs in their throats. Now scientists at Indiana University and Leiden University in The Netherlands have shown for the first time that parrots, like humans, also can use their tongues to craft and shape sound. "This is the first direct evidence that parrots are able to use their large tongues to change the acoustic properties of their vocalizations," said IU Bloomington neurologist Roderick Suthers, who participated in the research. "The basic idea here, we believe, is that motor control of tongue movements is an important part of communication, just as it is in humans." It's known that to produce sound, a parrot uses its syrinx, a voice box organ nestled between the trachea and lungs. The lingering question has been: What happens to that sound as it moves up and out of the throat? Ornithologists and bird enthusiasts have long noticed that parrots bob their tongues back and forth while they vocalize, but it wasn't known whether the tongue motions contributed significantly to sound-making.
Keyword: Language
Link ID: 6070 - Posted: 09.07.2004